- Brief Report
- Open access
- Published:
Resting-state blink rate does not increase following very-light-intensity exercise, but individual variation predicts executive function enhancement levels
Journal of Physiological Anthropology volume 44, Article number: 10 (2025)
Abstract
Background
Acute physical exercise, even at a very-light-intensity, potentiates prefrontal cortex activation and improves executive function. The underlying circuit mechanisms in the brain remain poorly understood, though we speculate a potential involvement of arousal-related neuromodulatory systems. Recently, our rodent study demonstrated that exercise, even at light-intensity, activates the midbrain dopaminergic neurons. Resting-state spontaneous eye blink rate is linked to brain-arousal neural circuits, and potentially to those modulated by dopaminergic system. We hypothesized that neural substrates linked to resting-state eye blink rate contribute to the cognitive impact of acute very-light-intensity exercise.
Method
We analyzed data from a previous study with a renewed focus on resting-state eye blink rate. Twenty-four healthy young adults completed both 10 min of cycling (very-light-intensity exercise: 30% peak oxygen uptake) and rest conditions. Resting-state eye blink rate and Stroop task performance were measured before and after both exercise and resting control.
Results
Results showed no significant differences in eye blink rate changes between conditions. However, correlation analyses revealed that exercise-induced changes in resting-state eye blink rate were significantly associated with individual variations in Stroop task performance enhancement.
Conclusion
Very-light-intensity exercise does not elicit a consistent increase in eye blink rate after exercise. This finding does not support the involvement of a blink increase-linked neural substrate in enhancing executive function through very-light-intensity exercise. However, resting-state eye blink rate that is altered by exercise is predictive of executive function enhancement levels; this may serve as a novel contactless biomarker for predicting exercise benefits for brain health and cognition.
Background
An active lifestyle promotes brain health [1]. Acute and chronic exercise, even very-light-intensity exercise, can enhance cognitive health related to the prefrontal cortex [2, 3]. The ascending arousal system play an important role in prefrontal cortex cognitive function [4]. In considering neural dynamics induced by acute exercise for cognitive enhancement, we speculate the involvement of exercise-induced activation of the ascending arousal system, particularly brainstem catecholaminergic (dopaminergic (DA) and noradrenergic (NA)) neurons through our animal-to-humans translational research [5, 6]. However, the neurobiological mechanisms are elusive and useful biomarkers for this have not yet been identified in humans.
Eye-based measurements are economical and non-invasive, which is useful in exercise-cognition science [3, 7,8,9]. Brainstem arousal and DA agents modulate blink rate in monkeys and humans; therefore, resting-state spontaneous eye blink rate (rssEBR) has been discussed as a biomarker linked to DA-related behavior [10,11,12,13]. Human positron emission tomography (PET) studies show that striatal DA release affects rssEBR [12, 14, 15]. For example, rssEBR decreases in Parkinson's disease, restored by a DA precursor [16]. Although a more precise mechanistic understanding is needed [17], rssEBR remains a potential biomarker for exploring DA-regulated cognitive enhancement [18]. Our previous cross-sectional study showed that a higher baseline rssEBR is associated with higher aerobic fitness and superior executive function, suggesting that rssEBR may mediate a fitness-cognition link [9]. This finding leads to the hypothesis that rssEBR may both provide mechanistic insight into and provide a reliable, non-invasive biomarker for the impact of exercise on cognition, not only for the effects of chronic exercise, but also for acute exercise.
We aimed to explore whether acute very-light-intensity exercise increases rssEBR and, subsequently, whether increased rssEBR following exercise predicts prefrontal cognitive enhancement. Our previous research shows that very-light-intensity exercise improves executive function with left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (l-DLPFC) activity [3]. We also showed that pupil dynamics, a measure of the NA-linked arousal system, predict, while exercising, enhanced executive function [3]. The current study builds on our previous research by analyzing additional facets of our previous data to examine the association between rssEBR and enhanced executive function.
Materials and methods
We focused on rssEBR pre- and post-exercise, analyzing different facets of the data presented in Kuwamizu et al. [3]. Here we will briefly describe the original methods. Thirty-four healthy young adults, all native Japanese speakers, were recruited, of which 24 (3 females, mean age 21.7 years, SD = 1.2; 21 males, mean age 22.2 years, SD = 1.5) passed the screening (e.g., medication use, eye health condition, task comprehension) and participated in both 10 min of very-light-intensity exercise (30% \(\dot{V\!}_{\text{O}_{2\text{peak}}}\)) on a cycle ergometer and a resting control condition on separate days in a crossover design (Fig. 1). rssEBR was measured for 3 min before and after exercise.
Summary of experimental paradigm. A Experimental paradigm flow. A color-word Stroop task was done before and after exercise/rest. Lateral prefrontal cortex activation was measured using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). B The Japanese version of the color-word-matching Stroop task. Participants responded with “yes” or “no” buttons, depending on whether the top font color matched the bottom color word or not. The task had 30 trials: 10 neutral, 10 congruent, and 10 incongruent, presented randomly. For neutral trials, the upper row contained crosses (XXX) in yellow, blue, green, or red, and the bottom row had color words written in black font. For congruent trials, the upper row had color words in colors that matched the meaning of the color word written in black in the bottom row. For incongruent trials, the upper row had color words in colors that did not match the meaning of the color word written in black in the bottom row to elicit cognitive conflict (i.e., Stroop interference)
Resting-state spontaneous eye blink rate
rssEBR was measured before (Pre1 before Stroop test; Pre2 after Stroop test but before exercise) and immediately after (Post) exercise (EX) and control (CTL) conditions (Fig. 1). The focus was on resting-states before and after exercise to avoid the influence of body movements on eyelid activity, unlike other previous studies that focused on pupil diameter during exercise. Participants looked at a black fixation cross on a gray digital screen 70 cm away. rssEBR (blinks per minute) was recorded for 3 min using a camcorder set above the monitor. Two researchers independently assessed rssEBR, and the average of their scores was used [9]. We previously confirmed the validity of this camcorder count using a vertical electrooculogram recording method [9]. Correlations between researchers exceeded r = 0.99. Individual rssEBR was calculated by dividing the total number of blinks during the 3-min interval by 3. All data were collected by 6:00 pm because rssEBR can be less stable at night [9, 11]. Participants were not informed that their blinks would be measured to ensure natural blinking.
Other variables
Executive function was assessed before and after exercise using a Stroop task (Fig. 1). Inverse efficiency scores (IES) were calculated as reaction time/accuracy. Stroop interference IES [incongruent IES–neutral IES] was calculated as inhibitory control [19, 20], the core component of executive function. Our previous study reported that very-light-intensity exercise significantly reduced Stroop interference compared to control. These measurements were used to explore rssEBR change relationships in the current analysis. For detailed methods and results, refer to Kuwamizu et al. [3]. Further exploratory analyses combining pupil diameter and l-DLPFC activity measured by functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) are addressed in Supplementary material 2: Extended data.
Results
Statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism V9; significance was P < 0.05.
The mean ± SD of rssEBR (blinks/min) was 32.8 ± 19.3, 34.9 ± 19.2, and 35.0 ± 19.5 for the EX and 32.0 ± 19.4, 27.8 ± 17.1, and 30.5 ± 19.1 for CTL, measured at Pre1, Pre2, and Post, respectively. Figure 2A shows the change in rssEBR across conditions. Our main hypothesis focused on differences before and after exercise and rest conditions, so we compared the immediate pre-/post-differences (Post–Pre2) for both conditions using a paired t-test, which indicated no significant condition differences (t(23) = 0.79, P = 0.44) (Fig. 2A).
rssEBR change and the association between rssEBR change and Stroop interference. A The differences in rssEBR for both CTL and EX conditions. Pre2 was selected as the baseline to compare the immediate pre- and post-exercise states, ensuring a direct assessment of exercise-induced changes. The box-and-whisker plot is drawn in the Tukey manner. Line plots represent individual data. B Association between rssEBR EX (Post–Pre2)–CTL (Post–Pre2) and Stroop interference EX (Post–Pre)–CTL (Post–Pre) The line in the scatter plot represents linear regression, the band represents 95% confidence. **P < 0.01, n.s.not significant
We then tested the relationship between rssEBR changes and Stroop task performance enhancement. To calculate the effect of very-light-intensity exercise on rssEBR, we computed the change in rssEBR as EX (Post–Pre2)–CTL (Post–Pre2). Similarly, for Stroop task performance, we analyzed the difference using EX (Post–Pre)–CTL (Post–Pre) [3]. The change of rssEBR EX(Post–Pre2)–CTL(Post–Pre2) significantly correlated with the reduction of Stroop interference EX(Post–Pre)–CTL(Post–Pre) (r (24) = − 0.62, P = 0.001) (Fig. 2B).
Discussion
We examined whether rssEBR, potentially involving brain DA modulation, explains the positive impact of acute very-light-intensity exercise on prefrontal executive function. Although very-light-intensity exercise did not elicit consistent rssEBR increase, there were significant associations between rssEBR variation and executive function enhancement post-exercise. These findings do not support the hypothesis that rssEBR-predicted neural substrates are the primary mechanism for cognitive enhancement. However, they do suggest that rssEBR change may predict executive function improvement levels following exercise.
These results do not support our hypothesis that very-light-intensity exercise increases rssEBR. Previous studies have produced mixed results; for example, a single bout of maximal aerobic exercise increased spontaneous EBR in adolescent boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) but not in girls with ADHD or typically developing children [21]. Other studies with smaller sample sizes (n = 16 or less) also failed to detect an increase in post-exercise blink rate, supporting our findings [22,23,24]. Here, we focused on rssEBR for 3 min immediately after exercise, but not during exercise. This was meant to exclude direct effects of physical and eye movements on blink rate. However, it is undeniable that DA upregulation may be eliminated immediately after exercise. Additionally, baseline rssEBR can vary depending on the study’s experimental conditions, such as visual stimuli. Although the baseline rssEBR was within the expected range and comparable to some previous data [12, 25], it tended to be higher overall compared to the average of previous studies [11], particularly on exercise condition days, which may reflect the motivational arousal to exercise. High baseline rssEBR may have negatively affected the detection of a potential exercise-induced increase in blink rates.
Interestingly, individual variations in rssEBR change highly correlated with improvements in executive function performance (Fig. 2B), which supports the hypothesis that rssEBR may provide mechanistic insight into and a useful biomarker of the impact of acute exercise on cognition. The direction of this correlation is similar to previous cross-sectional results showing that higher rssEBR is linked to better performance on the Stroop task [9]. Individual differences in exercise-induced DA release and receptor regulation may lead to variability in blink rate [12, 13], which, in turn, could predict post-exercise cognitive improvements.
Finally, the involvement of brain DA modulation in rssEBR remains uncertain [17]. Recently, it was reported that in humans, exercise induces the release of striatal endogenous DA correlated with the facilitation of simple decision-making reaction time [26]. Thus, to understand why rssEBR does not increase consistently with exercise, one speculative mechanism is the balance between D1 and D2 receptor activity [12] because their respective contributions to blink-rate change differ [27]. Other mechanisms, such as ocular factors, are also open for discussion.
Conclusion
Acute very-light-intensity exercise does not consistently increase rssEBR, thereby not indicating the involvement of a blink increase-linked neural substrate in enhancing executive function through very-light-intensity exercise. However, rssEBR increased by exercise is predictive of individual executive function enhancement levels. This implies that blink rate may provide a novel non-invasive biomarker for predicting the cognitive benefits of exercise in humans.
Data availability
All data that support the findings of this study are available upon request from the corresponding authors within the limits set by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the University of Tsukuba, which ensures that personal information will not be disclosed.
Abbreviations
- ADHD:
-
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
- CTL:
-
Control condition
- DA:
-
Dopamine
- DLPFC:
-
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
- EX:
-
Exercise condition
- fNIRS:
-
Functional near-infrared spectroscopy
- IES:
-
Inverse Efficiency Score
- NA:
-
Noradrenaline
- PET:
-
Positron emission tomography
- rssEBR:
-
Resting-State Spontaneous Eye Blink Rate
- SD:
-
Standard deviation
- \(\dot{V\!}_{\text{O}_{2\text{peak}}}\) :
-
Peak oxygen uptake
References
Dishman RK, Berthoud H-R, Booth FW, Cotman CW, Edgerton VR, Fleshner MR, et al. Neurobiology of exercise. Obesity. 2006;14:345–56.
Byun K, Hyodo K, Suwabe K, Fukuie T, Ha M-S, Damrongthai C, et al. Mild exercise improves executive function with increasing neural efficiency in the prefrontal cortex of older adults. Geroscience. 2024;46:309–25.
Kuwamizu R, Yamazaki Y, Aoike N, Hiraga T, Hata T, Yassa MA, et al. Pupil dynamics during very light exercise predict benefits to prefrontal cognition. Neuroimage. 2023;277:120244.
Cools R, Arnsten AFT. Neuromodulation of prefrontal cortex cognitive function in primates: the powerful roles of monoamines and acetylcholine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:309–28.
Yamazaki Y, Suwabe K, Nagano-Saito A, Saotome K, Kuwamizu R, Hiraga T, et al. A possible contribution of the locus coeruleus to arousal enhancement with mild exercise: evidence from pupillometry and neuromelanin imaging. Cereb Cortex Commun. 2023;4:tgad010.
Hiraga T, Hata T, Soya S, Shimoda R, Takahashi K, Soya M, et al. Light-exercise-induced dopaminergic and noradrenergic stimulation in the dorsal hippocampus: Using a rat physiological exercise model. FASEB J. 2024;38:e70215.
Zou L, Herold F, Ludyga S, Kamijo K, Müller NG, Pontifex MB, et al. Look into my eyes: What can eye-based measures tell us about the relationship between physical activity and cognitive performance? J Sport Health Sci. 2023;12:568–91.
Kuwamizu R, Yamazaki Y, Aoike N, Ochi G, Suwabe K, Soya H. Pupil-linked arousal with very light exercise: pattern of pupil dilation during graded exercise. J Physiol Sci. 2022;72:23.
Kuwamizu R, Suwabe K, Damrongthai C, Fukuie T, Ochi G, Hyodo K, et al. Spontaneous eye blink rate connects missing link between aerobic fitness and cognition. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2021;53:1425–33.
Demiral ŞB, Kure Liu C, Benveniste H, Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Activation of brain arousal networks coincident with eye blinks during resting state. Cereb Cortex. 2023;33:6792–802.
Jongkees BJ, Colzato LS. Spontaneous eye blink rate as predictor of dopamine-related cognitive function-A review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:58–82.
Demiral ŞB, Manza P, Biesecker E, Wiers C, Shokri-Kojori E, McPherson K, et al. Striatal D1 and D2 receptor availability are selectively associated with eye-blink rates after methylphenidate treatment. Commun Biol. 2022;5:1015.
Imburgio MJ, Ballard HK, Cornwall AC, Worthy DA, Bernard JA, Orr JM. Preliminary effects of prefrontal tDCS on dopamine-mediated behavior and psychophysiology. Behav Brain Res. 2021;402:113091.
Verhoeff NPLG, Christensen BK, Hussey D, Lee M, Papatheodorou G, Kopala L, et al. Effects of catecholamine depletion on D2 receptor binding, mood, and attentiveness in humans: a replication study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;74:425–32.
Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Gunn RN, Baker GB, Diksic M, et al. Modeling sensitization to stimulants in humans: an [11C]raclopride/positron emission tomography study in healthy men. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:1386–95.
Bologna M, Fasano A, Modugno N, Fabbrini G, Berardelli A. Effects of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation and L-DOPA on blinking in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Neurol. 2012;235:265–72.
Sescousse G, Ligneul R, van Holst RJ, Janssen LK, de Boer F, Janssen M, et al. Spontaneous eye blink rate and dopamine synthesis capacity: preliminary evidence for an absence of positive correlation. Eur J Neurosci. 2018;47:1081–6.
Luckey AM, McLeod LS, Huang Y, Mohan A, Vanneste S. Making memories last using the peripheral effect of direct current stimulation. Elife. 2023;12:e75586.
Schroeter ML, Zysset S, Wahl M, von Cramon DY. Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development–an event-related fNIRS study. Neuroimage. 2004;23:1317–25.
Kim K, Bohnen NI, Müller MLTM, Lustig C. Compensatory dopaminergic-cholinergic interactions in conflict processing: Evidence from patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage. 2019;190:94–106.
Tantillo M, Kesick CM, Hynd GW, Dishman RK. The effects of exercise on children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2002;34:203–12.
Ochi G, Kuwamizu R, Fujimoto T, Ikarashi K, Yamashiro K, Sato D. The effects of acute virtual reality exergaming on mood and executive function: exploratory crossover trial. JMIR Serious Games. 2022;10:e38200.
Hove MJ, Martinez SA, Shorrock SR. Physical exercise increases perceived musical pleasure: Modulatory roles of arousal, affect, or dopamine? Psychol Music. 2022;50:849–61.
O’Connor PJ, Petruzzello SJ. Spontaneous eyeblinks and state anxiety following exercise. Int J Neurosci. 1992;62:57–63.
Nakano T. The right angular gyrus controls spontaneous eyeblink rate: a combined structural MRI and TMS study. Cortex. 2017;88:186–91.
Ando S, Fujimoto T, Sudo M, Watanuki S, Hiraoka K, Takeda K, et al. The neuromodulatory role of dopamine in improved reaction time by acute cardiovascular exercise. J Physiol. 2024;602:461–84.
Kotani M, Kiyoshi A, Murai T, Nakako T, Matsumoto K, Matsumoto A, et al. The dopamine D1 receptor agonist SKF-82958 effectively increases eye blinking count in common marmosets. Behav Brain Res. 2016;300:25–30.
Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful to H. Imoarai, C. Damrongthai, A. Nagano-Saito, M. Okamoto, T. Hiraga, and T. Hata (University of Tsukuba, Japan) for data collection support and discussion. They thank H. Koizumi (University of Tsukuba, Japan) for blink analysis program. They express their gratitude to M. Noguchi (ELCS English Language Consultation Services, Japan) for helping with the manuscript. The illustration (cycling man in Fig. 1) was contributed by C. Damrongthai.
Author’s Note
We clarify that this study uses a subset of data from our previous research (Kuwamizu et al., 2023), yet offers a distinct focus and novel findings. By introducing this new facet of the connection between blink rate and cognitive function, we aim to extend our understanding of the mechanisms of and identify a new biomarker for cognitive enhancement through very-light-intensity exercise. Unlike our previous study, which focused on pupil dynamics measured during exercise, this study measures rssEBR before and after exercise, providing a different purpose and perspective. We acknowledge and declare the secondary use of our prior work. We hope that this paper will be considered in conjunction with our previous findings, preventing an overestimation of our academic achievements and meta-analysis results. This submission adheres to the criteria for the "Brief Report" section in this journal.
Funding
This work was supported in part by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) 21H04858 (H.S.), 20J20893 (R.K.), 23KJ1169 (R.K.),22H00494 (H.S.), 24H00670 (H.S.) and 24K20598 (R.K.); the Japan Science and Technology Agency (JST) Grant JPMJMI19D5 (H.S.); and the Meiji Yasuda Life Foundation of Health and Welfare Grant (R.K.). This work was also supported in part by the Inviting Overseas Educational Research Units in University of Tsukuba (2016–2023) (to H.S.).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Ryuta Kuwamizu: Conceptualization, methodology, conducting experiments, data analysis, visualization, writing (original draft preparation), project administration, and funding acquisition; Yudai Yamazaki: Conducting experiments, data analysis, and writing (reviewing and editing); Naoki Aoike: Conducting experiments, data analysis, writing (reviewing and editing); Dongmin Lee: Data analysis, writing (reviewing and editing); Hideaki Soya: Supervision, conceptualization, methodology, writing (reviewing and editing), and funding acquisition.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the University of Tsukuba and was carried out in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki, except for preregistration. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Material 1: Sensitivity analysis.
Supplemental Material 2: Extended data.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Kuwamizu, R., Yamazaki, Y., Aoike, N. et al. Resting-state blink rate does not increase following very-light-intensity exercise, but individual variation predicts executive function enhancement levels. J Physiol Anthropol 44, 10 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s40101-025-00390-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s40101-025-00390-x